Children benefit from morphological relatedness independently of orthographic relatedness when they learn to spell new words
Autor: | Ronald Peereman, Sébastien Pacton, Jean-Noël Foulin, Marion Nys, Rebecca Treiman, Amandine Afonso Jaco |
---|---|
Přispěvatelé: | Laboratoire de psychologie:Santé et qualité de vie, Université Bordeaux Segalen - Bordeaux 2 |
Jazyk: | angličtina |
Rok vydání: | 2018 |
Předmět: |
Male
Morphology Implicit learning media_common.quotation_subject Orthographic learning [SHS.PSY]Humanities and Social Sciences/Psychology Experimental and Cognitive Psychology Spelling 050105 experimental psychology Phonetics Reading (process) Developmental and Educational Psychology Humans Learning 0501 psychology and cognitive sciences Child media_common Language 4. Education 05 social sciences Orthographic projection Spell Linguistics Reading Self-teaching Female Suffix Psychology Spelling acquisition 050104 developmental & child psychology |
Zdroj: | Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Elsevier, 2018, 171, pp.71-83. ⟨10.1016/j.jecp.2018.02.003⟩ |
ISSN: | 0022-0965 1096-0457 |
DOI: | 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.02.003⟩ |
Popis: | We examined whether French children in Grades 3 and 5 (aged ∼ 8–11 years) benefit from morphological relatedness beyond orthographic relatedness in the implicit learning of new spellings. Children silently read stories that included two target nonwords. One nonword was in an opaque condition in that nothing in the story could justify the spelling of its final sound. The other nonword was in either a morphological condition (for children in the morphological group) or an orthographic condition (for children in the orthographic group). In the morphological condition, the final spelling of the target nonword was justified by two morphologically related nonwords. For example, coirardage, obtained by adding the suffix age to coirard, designates the coirard’s song and justifies the final silent d of coirard. The orthographic condition included two nonwords that were orthographically but not morphologically related to the target. For example, the coirard’s song was coirardume, obtained by adding ume, which is not a suffix, to coirard. Then, 30 min after reading the stories, children were asked to choose the correct spelling of each nonword from among three phonologically plausible alternatives (e.g., coirard, coirars, coirar). In the morphological group, both third and fifth graders more often selected the correct spellings for items presented in the morphological condition than for items presented in the opaque condition. In the orthographic group, the results were very similar in the opaque and orthographic conditions. The findings show that the benefit of morphological relatedness in the implicit learning of new spellings cannot be reduced to orthographic relatedness. |
Databáze: | OpenAIRE |
Externí odkaz: |